Bees are coffee addicts too

Bee coffee adicted

As the Hungarian mathematician Alfréd Rényi famously put it (although usually misattributed to Paul Erdös), mathematicians are devices for turning coffee into theorems. Other people drink coffee for a variety of reasons, and considering that coffee is very far from being the only popular beverage containing caffeine, it is not difficult to believe that caffeine is one of the most consumed drugs in the world.

The scientific consensus is that caffeine and other drugs are produced by plants as a defense against herbivores. However, the production of a very low concentration of caffeine in the nectar of certain plants such as lemon, pomelo and grapefruit trees do not seem to agree well with this explanation. Geraldine Wright and her colleagues of Newcastle University, University of Dundee and Arizona State University found small concentrations of caffeine in the nectar of four species of Citrus and three species of Coffea. In a recent article 1 they explore the possible ecological function and concluded that pollinators, like human mathematicians do, might get a boost in memory formation from caffeine.

Figure 1. Bee extending its proboscis. | Credit: Neerav Bhat
Figure 1. Bee extending its proboscis. | Credit: Neerav Bhat

Pavlov’s dog learned to associate the sound of a bell with food. Once trained, it drooled after the sound of a bell, even when there was no food present. Bees can also be trained to associate certain scents with feeding. The bee version for drooling is the extension of the proboscis when the bee detects the odour it has been trained to associate with food. Geraldine Wright and her colleagues trained individual honeybees to associate a certain floral odor with a solution of sugar (sucrose) and different small doses of caffeine.

Figure 2. A) Mean caffeine concentration in the nectar of three species of Coffea and three species of Citrus range from 0.003 to 0.253 mM, the last figure being comparable to the typical concentration in a cup of instant coffee. B) Bees reject a sweet solution containing high concentrations of caffeine but will usually accept nectar with concentrations similar to those found naturally in Citrus and Coffea nectar.
Figure 2. A) Mean caffeine concentration in the nectar of three species of Coffea and three species of Citrus range from 0.003 to 0.253 mM, the last figure being comparable to the typical concentration in a cup of instant coffee. B) Bees reject a sweet solution containing high concentrations of caffeine but will usually accept nectar with concentrations similar to those found naturally in Citrus and Coffea nectar. | Credit: Wright et al (2013)

The researchers found that honeybees have cells in their mouth parts that are sensitive to the flavour of caffeine. The bees reject solutions with concentrations of caffeine higher than 1mM but, when presented with the smaller concentrations normally present in the nectar of Citrus and Coffea, bees happily take a sip (Figure 2). When comparing the effect of different small concentrations of caffeine in learning, the researchers found that it hardly affected the learning process. However, there was a surprisingly big effect in long term memory: bees trained with a solution of sugar and caffeine were almost three times as likely to remember the scent than bees trained with a similarly sweet but decaff solution (Figure 3).

Figure 3 A) The rate of learning of bees to associate a certain scent to a sweet reward is only slightly affected by small concentrations of caffeine. B) Results of a test measuring the ability to remember the scent for bees trained 10 mins before (white bars) or 24h before (red and brown bars). With caffeine concentrations in the nectar during training higher than 1e-7 (red bars), bees were more than twice more likely to remember the stimulus in the long term. Dashed red bars indicate the concentrations typically found in Citrus and Coffea flower nectar. | Credit: Wright et al (2013)
Figure 3 A) The rate of learning of bees to associate a certain scent to a sweet reward is only slightly affected by small concentrations of caffeine. B) Results of a test measuring the ability to remember the scent for bees trained 10 mins before (white bars) or 24h before (red and brown bars). With caffeine concentrations in the nectar during training higher than 1e-7 (red bars), bees were more than twice more likely to remember the stimulus in the long term. Dashed red bars indicate the concentrations typically found in Citrus and Coffea flower nectar. | Credit: Wright et al (2013)

In mammals, the neurological effect of caffeine is to prevent the neurotrasmitter adenosine to bind to certain receptors. In particular, caffeine blocking of adenosine produces long-term activation in certain areas of the hippocampus involved in memory formation. In collaboration with Dr Mary Palmer at the University of Dundee, the researchers explored the effect of caffeine in cells of the mushroom body (a structure in the insect brain that plays a role in olfactory memory and learning) and they discovered that small concentrations of caffeine produced a depolarization of the membrane of Kenyon cells. This depolarization also appeared when putting a solution of DPCPX, another adenosine antagonist, so it is likely that caffeine acts in the mushroom body in the same way as in the mammalian hippocampus: blocking adenosine receptors. The depolarization of Kenyon cells in the mushroom body is likely to lead to an increase of excitability, which may facilitate memory formation.

While still acting as a repellent at high doses, such as the ones found in coffee beans, this study shows that flowers could in fact be producing low doses of caffeine to secretly drug pollinators and modify their behaviour. This newly discovered advance in the arms race of plants would make it easier for bees to associate a determinate species of flower with reward, making it more likely that the same bee will visit the same species in the future and thus improving the chances of pollination. And this is maybe not theorem production, but still a very important ecological function.

References

  1. Wright G.A., Baker D.D., Palmer M.J., Stabler D., Mustard J.A., Power E.F., Borland A.M. & Stevenson P.C. (2013). Caffeine in Floral Nectar Enhances a Pollinator’s Memory of Reward, Science, 339 (6124) 1202-1204. DOI: 10.1126/science.1228806

Written by

1 comment

Leave a Reply

Your email address will not be published.Required fields are marked *